Daniel Willingham--Science & Education
Hypothesis non fingo
  • Home
  • About
  • Books
  • Articles
  • Op-eds
  • Videos
  • Learning Styles FAQ
  • Daniel Willingham: Science and Education Blog

Media multitasking and cognition in teens--new data

6/7/2016

 
Several reports in the last few years have shown a relationship between cognitive function and the frequency of media multitasking (e.g., Cain et al, 2014; Minear et al, 2013; Ophir et al, 2009). Studies don’t always directly replicate each measure, but overall people who report habitual media multitasking tend to be less proficient in the use of working memory, and more impulsive. 

One drawback of this work is that it has tested only college students, although they are obviously not alone in multitasking. As technology prices drop, younger children have increased access to mp3 players, smart phones, gaming platforms, etc., and with increased access comes increased use. Is the relationship between multitasking habits and cognition different for younger children?

In a recent study, Matthew Cain and colleagues (in press) tested 73 subjects (average age = 14.4 years) on a battery of cognitive and personality tests. They largely replicated previous work, and added some interesting extensions.

First, they found that frequency of media multitasking was negatively associated with working memory span measures. They did not find a relationship with the ability to filter information in working memory, which contrasts with some previous work (Ophir et al, 2009).  Cain et al speculated that Ophir et al may have used a filtering task that was also demanding of span.

Second, Cain et al wanted to use a measure that had more real-world validity, so they correlated media multitasking scores with student scores on the state tests for math in English (for Massachusetts, where the work was conducted). A significant negative correlation was observed. Scores were not related to standard lab measures of math and English ability, however, leading the researchers to suggest that scores on the state test might have been affected because test administration is longer, so scores would more likely be affected if students were prone to distraction.  In other words, students might have been equally proficient in math and English, but less able to maintain attention for a long duration.

Third, researchers wanted to show that media multitasking was not related to cognition across the board, so they administered some tests of cognitive ability they thought were unlikely to be related, and indeed found that manual dexterity, and the unconscious learning of a complex category, were not related to media multitasking.

Fourth, researchers tested whether media multitasking was related to personality characteristics and beliefs that are known to be related to academic achievement. They observed no relationship with grit or conscientiousness, but did see a robust negative relationship with growth mindset. The authors expected to see relationships of all three with media multitasking. Why is not clear to me. If you think that most teens know that multitasking while reading or studying is not conducive to good performance, you might predict a negative correlation with grit and conscientiousness, but it’s not clear that most teens are aware of the relationship. The observed relationship with growth mindset—a belief about the nature of intelligence—puzzles me, and the authors say little about it.

Perhaps the most important aspect of these results is the age of the children tested. The results show that the relationship between media multitasking and cognition that we see in college students is observable by middle adolescence. That marginally increases the probability that the direction of causality is cognition prompting media multitasking behavior, not multitasking prompting changes in cognition. The authors are appropriately cautious in discussing causality but the results are a bit easier to account for with that explanation, the idea being that younger children have had less multitasking experience, and so there has been less opportunity for multitasking to influence cognition—yet the relationship is present.

One point about this study does make me uneasy. The median self-reported estimate of media use was 18 hours/day. A good number of subjects offered weekly estimates of media use that amounted to more than 24 hours per day. The authors note that these seem like overestimates, and so they suggest that these raw figures are not really interpretable.
​
But they go on to take the subject reports of frequency of media multitasking at face value. For each medium, subjects were asked how often they engaged with that medium while doing something else, choosing among four categories (Never, a little of the time, some of the time, most of the time). Why trust these estimates, if their estimates of the number of hours spent with media are not trustworthy? Both are retrospective memory reports, and the judgment of media multitasking has not been validated, as far as I can tell.
​
That validation ought to be high on the priority list for researchers in this area. It would be a terrible shame if all of these effects that we are tying to media multitasking actually ought to be tied to accuracy and bias in memory reports. 
Kenneth Sheck
6/7/2016 09:00:14 am

If cognition prompts media multi-tasking behavior, what does that mean? That some children are born to be tempted to multi-task? Or that the way some children are raised caused them to be more likely to multi-task?

Children are handed screens mighty young these days. Could that have an influence on their propensity to multi-task?

Finally, what's the cautious take-away for teachers? There's something of a battle going on in my school - some teachers allow students to listen to music on their phones while the students are doing solo work. Other teachers, like me, feel that just having the phone out is something of a distraction.

If we're to get students to engage in "deliberate practice," shouldn't we be promoting focus?

Daniel Willingham
6/8/2016 09:34:33 am

Kenneth, great questions, and I don't think there are clear research-based answers to any of them.

regarding the question "should children be allowed to listen to music while they work?" my read of this litearture is that every task you add takes away attention and prompts a cost to the primary task...but that's sometimes NOT true for music. There are a lot of studies on the effect of listening to music while you work and the data seem very inconsistent. Hence, when students claim they work better w/ music on, they very well might, but it seems to be an individual thing...some do, some don't. So best practice seems to be "if they want to work w/ music, let them do so....but watch 'em, and see if it helps, as they claim."


Comments are closed.

    Enter your email address:

    Delivered by FeedBurner

    RSS Feed


    Purpose

    The goal of this blog is to provide pointers to scientific findings that are applicable to education that I think ought to receive more attention.

    Archives

    April 2022
    July 2020
    May 2020
    March 2020
    February 2020
    December 2019
    October 2019
    April 2019
    March 2019
    January 2019
    October 2018
    September 2018
    August 2018
    June 2018
    March 2018
    February 2018
    November 2017
    October 2017
    September 2017
    August 2017
    July 2017
    June 2017
    April 2017
    March 2017
    February 2017
    November 2016
    September 2016
    August 2016
    July 2016
    June 2016
    May 2016
    April 2016
    December 2015
    July 2015
    April 2015
    March 2015
    January 2015
    September 2014
    August 2014
    July 2014
    June 2014
    May 2014
    April 2014
    March 2014
    February 2014
    January 2014
    December 2013
    November 2013
    October 2013
    September 2013
    August 2013
    July 2013
    June 2013
    May 2013
    April 2013
    March 2013
    February 2013
    January 2013
    December 2012
    November 2012
    October 2012
    September 2012
    August 2012
    July 2012
    June 2012
    May 2012
    April 2012
    March 2012
    February 2012

    Categories

    All
    21st Century Skills
    Academic Achievement
    Academic Achievement
    Achievement Gap
    Adhd
    Aera
    Animal Subjects
    Attention
    Book Review
    Charter Schools
    Child Development
    Classroom Time
    College
    Consciousness
    Curriculum
    Data Trustworthiness
    Education Schools
    Emotion
    Equality
    Exercise
    Expertise
    Forfun
    Gaming
    Gender
    Grades
    Higher Ed
    Homework
    Instructional Materials
    Intelligence
    International Comparisons
    Interventions
    Low Achievement
    Math
    Memory
    Meta Analysis
    Meta-analysis
    Metacognition
    Morality
    Motor Skill
    Multitasking
    Music
    Neuroscience
    Obituaries
    Parents
    Perception
    Phonological Awareness
    Plagiarism
    Politics
    Poverty
    Preschool
    Principals
    Prior Knowledge
    Problem-solving
    Reading
    Research
    Science
    Self-concept
    Self Control
    Self-control
    Sleep
    Socioeconomic Status
    Spatial Skills
    Standardized Tests
    Stereotypes
    Stress
    Teacher Evaluation
    Teaching
    Technology
    Value-added
    Vocabulary
    Working Memory